Cognitive Load Theory

Photo by Anna Shvets on Pexels.com

By Sarah Stevenson, MA- The Tini Yogini

Educational psychologist John Sweller is the father of cognitive load theory (CLT), which describes the amount of effort and how much working memory one can hold at any one period of time (Sweller, 1988). CLT is based on the human information processing model that includes sensory memory, short-term memory, working memory, and long-term memory and how these parts work together (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968). Research in CLT is incredibly beneficial for educators, especially in this technological age when humans have so much competition for their attention and working memory. Here is a detailed look at the brain according to cognitive load theory and the human information processing model.

Photo by cottonbro on Pexels.com

Individuals take in information from the world around them through the senses, and memories are formed, or information is discarded during this interaction. Sensory memory is processed quickly through the nervous system and consists of iconic (seeing), haptic (touch), echoic (hearing), olfactory (smell), and gustatory (taste) (Pratte, 2018; Sperling, 1960). If the stimuli presented is recognized, it will move to the short-term memory, if it is not, it will be lost and forgotten.

Once the stimuli pass through the senses, they move to the short-term (primary, active) memory, where the mind can hold information and keep it accessible for a short period of time, ranging from about fifteen to thirty seconds (Paas & Sweller, 2012). According to research, short-term memory can only hold about seven pieces of information, give or take one or two (Cowen, 2008; Miller, 1956). If short-term memories have any chance of moving to the long term, they must be actively preserved through focused attention and rehearsal (Ginns & Leppink, 2019).

Photo by Suzy Hazelwood on Pexels.com

Long-term memory stores information for an indefinite extended time frame. Information stored in the long-term memory will be recalled anywhere from hours, days, or even years after it is processed (Sweller, 1988). Long-term memory stores this information in schemas. Schemas are thought patterns and memories that have been chunked into categories by the working memory to simplify storage in the long-term memory bank (Paas & Sweller, 2012). Individuals begin to build these schemas from an early age, and they continue to form and change throughout a lifetime. Schemas help humans understand the world around them by drawing on past experiences to help them understand a novel experience. The working memory sometimes accesses schemas automatically because this is not done with a conscious effort the working memory does not feel the load (Geary, 2002). Schemas can also negatively impact an individual through stereotyping and getting locked into phobias and fears (Goodboy et al., 2016).

Working memory has the limited ability to store information and is responsible for decision making and reasoning (Alloway, & Alloway, 2013). The working memory decides what will move to the long-term memory and what will be forgotten and discarded. Working memory can also pull information from the long-term memory and apply it to situations at hand. Cognitive load is the amount of information the working memory can hold.

This cognitive load can show up in an intrinsic form related to the complexity of a topic and whether or not learning can connect to pre-formed schemas in the mind. Cognitive load can also show up in an extrinsic form which relates to how information is being offered to the learner and can include exterior components like the noise level of an environment and the amount of material being presented. Last is a germane cognitive load that refers to the amount of work (how easy or difficult) one must carry out to create schemas with the presented information (Klepsch et al., 2017).

Biological primary information like face recognition (Bentin et al., 1999) and learning to speak the native language comes automatically (Kuhl, 2000), puts no cognitive load on the working memory, and is considered an evolutionary strength to carrying on the species. Biological secondary information (culturally important, not acquired automatically through evolution) requires conscious effort to obtain and retain knowledge in the long-term memory and creates large amounts of cognitive load on the working memory (Geary, 2002).

As seen in Figure 1 below, environmental stimuli are presented; the stimuli pass through the senses (sight, hearing, smell, touch, and taste) and are processed by the nervous system that transfers the information to short-term memory. If the stimuli are focused on and rehearsed or align with a schema that currently exists, they will move to the long-term memory, where the information can be accessed in working memory for hours, days or years to come (Barefoot TEFL Teacher, n.d.).

Benefits and Research of Cognitive Load Theory

            Cognitive Load Theory benefits humans in many ways. CLT helps education because it considers designing a curriculum that includes only the necessary pieces of information. Research shows that working memory is expanded when learning draws on existing schemas (knowledge), allowing for a deeper understanding of what would typically be complex information (van Kesteren & Meeter, 2020). Clear, straightforward lesson plans enable the brain to use the least amount of effort by building on pre-formed schemas (van Kesteren & Meeter, 2020). Teachers must learn to present only the information directly related to learning to avoid overloading the working memory (Wilson, 2019).

Photo by Pixabay on Pexels.com

Educators can create better learning environments, and employers can create better work environments by administering assessments to learn the individuals’ strengths and expertise to increase working memory that draw from pre-formed schemas (Ogba et al., 2020). One of Bandura’s tools for self-efficacy is mastery of skills, individuals who can build on pre-formed strengths will have a symbiotic relationship with the tasks at hand that will increase self-confidence (Bandura, 1977). The goal is to work smarter, not harder and CLT offers this.

Current research suggests less load can be placed on the working memory by decreasing the number of competing stimuli for attention in one’s environment (Ford, 2016). New and exciting classroom designs are rising in education to benefit the learner (Teach Thought, n.d). When a classroom has a poor design, and too many stimuli are competing for a person’s sensory attention, learning opportunities are lost (Ginns & Leppink, 2019). Controlling the climate, air quality, offering a clean, organized environment, and appropriate lighting will benefit an individual’s learning experience.

Photo by Eileen lamb on Pexels.com

Innovative researchers impassioned to create new ways to elucidate learning are beginning to apply CLT concepts in their explorations. A recent study (Li, 2020) looked at priming students for flexible thinking by decreasing cognitive fixation brought on by past schemas (Smith, 2003) with the tool of encouraging ‘magic thinking’ to incite the imagination. Creative thinking is associated with a flexible mindset that allows individuals to access new ideas when faced with unfamiliar encounters, decreasing anxiety and expanding learning opportunities to develop long-term memory (Middleton, 2015).

Limitations and Challenges of Cognitive Load Theory

 Cognitive load theory is an incredible tool for streamlining learning and processing information when understood and applied appropriately, but the theory is not void of flaws. According to research, it is nearly impossible to measure how much cognitive load any one person can hold, which creates an issue with internal bias and external validity (Naismith & Cavalcanti, 2015; Rahi, 2017). It is difficult to generalize results gained from a research study if each person has a unique level of cognitive load tolerance (Patino & Ferreira, 2018).

Photo by Daria Shevtsova on Pexels.com

Educators today have the specific challenge of balancing overcrowded classrooms because of budget cuts and overpopulation (Jackson et al., 2020). It is unfair to place the task of building individual education plans for each student’s specific cognitive load on the teacher’s shoulders. The amount of responsibility placed on teachers today is causing them to retire and choose other career paths, and today’s children will be negatively affected by this (Oberle et al., 2020; Su et al., 2020).

Building on the concept that each person’s cognitive load capacity is unique, it is unfair to overgeneralize CLT to all individuals. Less cognitive load in certain humans may allow for better learning opportunities, while less cognitive load for others may decrease the amount of learning that is taking place (Ford, 2016). What is suitable for one is not necessarily good for all.

CLT must be paired with other collaborative learning techniques if it has any chance of having a quantifiable positive relationship in a classroom setting (Kirschner et al., 2018). The theory is limited in that it is unable to stand alone in its application to group settings. Further research needs to move towards the natural environment and finding the best teaching strategies that can collaborate with CLT to build the most generalizable learning tools for students in all degrees of education (Chen et al., 2018; Mancinetti et al., 2019).

Conclusion

Cognitive load theory opens one’s eyes to the pitfalls and abilities of the human processing system (Sweller, 1988). Knowing how the brain takes in material from the senses, allows or disallows it into the short-term memory, passing it on to the working memory and depositing it into the long-term memory or discarding the information entirely is a fascinating system that needs to be studied deeper. With an adequate understanding of the theory, one could help others through education, therapy, and training to live their best lives in a conscious, well-thought-out way.

References

Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. (2013). Working memory: the connected intelligence. Psychology Press.

Atkinson, R.C. & Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In K.W. Spence & J.T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press. Pp. 89– 195. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0079742108604223

Barefoot TEFL Teacher. (n.d.) Information processing model. https://www.barefootteflteacher.com/blog/what-is-cognitive-load-theory

Bentin, S., Deouell, L. Y., & Soroker, N. (1999). Selective visual streaming in face recognition: Evidence from developmental prosopagnosia. Neuroreport, 10, 823–827. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10208555/

Chen, O., Castro-Alonso, J.C., Paas, F. & Sweller, J. (2018) Extending cognitive load theory to incorporate working memory resource depletion: Evidence from the spacing effect. Educational Psychology Review 30,483–501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9426-2

Cowan, N. (2008). Chapter 20 What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323–338. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00020-9

Ford, A. (2016). Planning classroom design and layout to increase pedagogical options for secondary teachers. Educational Planning, 23(1), 25–33. https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy1.ncu.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ1208206&site=eds-live

Geary, D. C. (2002). Principles of evolutionary educational psychology. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 317–345. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S1041608002000468

Ginns, P., & Leppink, J. (2019). Special issue on cognitive load theory: Editorial. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 255–259. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1007/s10648-019-09474-4

Goodboy, A. K., Martin, M. M., & Rittenour, C. E. (2016). Bullying as an expression of intolerant schemas. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 9(4), 277–282. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1007/s40653-016-0089-9

Jackson, C. K., Wigger, C., & Heyu Xiong. (2020). The costs of cutting school spending: Lessons from the great recession. Education Next, 20(4), 1–12. https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy1.ncu.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edb&AN=146884556&site=eds-live

Klepsch, M., Schmitz, F. & Seufert, T. (2017). Development and validation of two instruments measuring intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01997

Kuhl, P. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 11850–11857. https://www.pnas.org/content/97/22/11850

Li, T. (2020). Use of magic performance as a schema disruption method to facilitate flexible thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 38, 1-13. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100735

Mancinetti, M., Guttormsen, S., & Berendonk, C. (2019). Cognitive load in internal medicine: What every clinical teacher should know about cognitive load theory. European Journal of Internal Medicine, 60, 4–8. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1016/j.ejim.2018.08.013

Middleton, A. (2015). Thinking about smart learning. Smart learning, 15. https://www.academia.edu/12512765/Smart_learning_teaching_and_learning_with_smartphones_and_tablets_in_post_compulsory_education

Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The psychological review, 63, 81-97. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1957-02914-001

Naismith, L. M., & Cavalcanti, R. B. (2015). Validity of cognitive load measures in simulation-based training: A systematic review. Academic medicine: journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges90(11), 24–35. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26505098/

Oberle, E., Gist, A., Cooray, M. S., & Pinto, J. B. R. (2020). Do students notice stress in teachers? Associations between classroom teacher burnout and students’ perceptions of teacher social-emotional competence. Psychology in the Schools, 57(11), 1741-1747. https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy1.ncu.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edsgao&AN=edsgcl.637467470&site=eds-live

Ogba, F. N., Ugodulunwa, C. A., & Igu, N. C. N. (2020). Assessment of training needs of teachers and administrators for effective inclusive education delivery in secondary schools in southeast nigeria. International Journal of Educational Leadership Preparation, 15(1), 72–91. https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy1.ncu.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ1254503&site=eds-live

Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: Using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 27–45. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2

Patino, C. M., & Ferreira, J. C. (2018). Internal and external validity: can you apply research study results to your patients?. Jornal Brasileiro de Pneumologia : Publicacao Oficial da Sociedade Brasileira de Pneumologia e Tisilogia44(3), 183. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1806-37562018000000164

Rahi, S. (2017). Research design and methods: A systematic review of research paradigms, sampling issues, and instruments development. International Journal of Economics & Management Sciences6(2), 1-5. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/316701205_Research_Design_and_Methods_A_Systematic_Review_of_Research_Paradigms_Sampling_Issues_and_Instruments_Development

Smith, S. M. (2003). The constraining effects of initial ideas. In Paulus P. B, & Nijstad, B. A. (Eds.), Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration (p. 15–31). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195147308.003.0002

Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological monographs: General and applied, 74(11), 1. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2011-17733-001

Su, Z., Gai, X., Jiang, M., & Chi, X. (2020). A study on the relationship between job demand control-support and job burnout of college english teachers. 2020 International Conference on Public Health and Data Science (ICPHDS), Public Health and Data Science (ICPHDS), 2020 International Conference on, ICPHDS, 33–38. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1109/ICPHDS51617.2020.00013

Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem-solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/0364021388900237

Teach Thought. (n.d). 20 ways to set up a classroom to help your students think. https://www.teachthought.com/pedagogy/20-ways-to-setup-a-classroom-to-help-your-students-think/

van Kesteren, M. T. R., & Meeter, M. (2020). How to optimize knowledge construction in the brain. Npj Science of Learning, 5(1), 1-7. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1038/s41539-020-0064-y

Wilson, G. (2019). Ten quick tips for creating an effective lesson. PLoS Computational Biology, 15(4), e1006915. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006915

By The Tini Yogini

I hold a Masters degree in psychology with an emphasis in behavior modification & have worked with individuals in the public & private sector for 20 years. I am a certified Yoga Instructor RYT & sole proprietor of my own health and wellness business for the last 14 years built from the ground up. I implement Cognitive Behavior Therapy theories to help clients eliminate self-destructive unconscious behavior patterns and replace them with positive life affirming behavior patterns. I am also a professional writer and have spent the last 10 years writing articles about implementing research-based behavior strategies to positively enhance one’s life. I teach workshops throughout the United States that teach individuals how to flourish and thrive.

Leave a comment